KUKU

KU ScholarWorks

  • myKU
  • Email
  • Enroll & Pay
  • KU Directory
    • Login
    View Item 
    •   KU ScholarWorks
    • Dissertations and Theses
    • Dissertations
    • View Item
    •   KU ScholarWorks
    • Dissertations and Theses
    • Dissertations
    • View Item
    JavaScript is disabled for your browser. Some features of this site may not work without it.

    Considering Visual Stimuli as an Active Ingredient in Vocabulary Interventions with Children on the Autism Spectrum: Evidence from a Scoping Review

    Thumbnail
    View/Open
    Pitt_ku_0099D_17361_DATA_1.pdf (3.130Mb)
    Issue Date
    2020-08-31
    Author
    Pitt, Adrienne
    Publisher
    University of Kansas
    Format
    92 pages
    Type
    Dissertation
    Degree Level
    Ph.D.
    Discipline
    Intercampus Program in Communicative Disorders
    Rights
    Copyright held by the author.
    Metadata
    Show full item record
    Abstract
    Purpose: Children with ASD demonstrate different visual processing profiles than typically developing children (Walton & Ingersoll, 2013; DiCriscio & Troiani, 2018). Evidence suggests that children with ASD may demonstrate overselectivity (i.e. overgeneralizing labels to other objects), weak central coherence (i.e. heightened local over global focus), and difficulty inhibiting distractor information, all which negatively impact word learning. These theories, as well as other evidence, suggest that children with ASD may perform best with iconic visual stimuli, especially children with severe language impairment and severe ASD. Iconic stimuli are those that most closely represent the referent and may often include objects and color photographs, with less iconic stimuli representing the referent more loosely, including line-drawings and clip-art. Language interventions targeting word learning use visual stimuli to teach word labels. However, children’s performance with various stimuli during these interventions has not been closely explored. Therefore, this scoping review was conducted to answer the following questions (1) Does visual stimuli selection in language intervention impact outcomes in children with ASD? If so, how; (2) Does the relationship between stimuli selection and language outcomes vary by the child’s pre-treatment language skills and/or ASD severity; and (3) Does intervention mode have an effect on type of stimuli selected? Methods: A scoping review protocol was developed based on previously reported frameworks for conducting scoping reviews (Arksey & O’Malley, 2005; Peters et al., 2015). This protocol followed a 5-stage process including: (1) identifying the research question, (2) identifying relevant studies, (3) study selection, (4) charting the data, and (5) collating, summarizing and reporting the results. Relevant studies were identified through databases searches using the pre-determined search strategy. Initial searching resulted in 794 identified studies. After screening, eligibility checks and ancestral searches were complete, 18 studies were selected for inclusion in this study. To answer the study questions, each study was coded across the following parameters: (1) author(s), (2) year of publication, (3) study design, (4) # of participants, (5) participant language level, (6) participant age range, (7) ASD severity, (8) intervention type, (9) target of intervention (10) response mode of communication, (11) visual stimuli used, (12) number of words taught, (13) number of words learned, (14) number of sessions, and (15) rate of learning. Results: In the included studies, different types of visual stimuli were used, with 7 studies including line-drawings, 7 including photographs and 7 including objects. The average rate of learning was highest for those that included photographs, with the next highest for those that used line-drawings, with the lowest rate of learning from those that use objects. When split up by participant language ability, results showed that (1) children who are nonverbal had the highest rate of learning with objects (M = 1.6), (2) children who are minimally verbal had the highest rate of learning with photographs (M = 1.43), (3) children who have emerging verbal skills had the highest rate of learning with line-drawings (M =2.13). Only one study included children with average verbal abilities, which demonstrated a rate of learning of 4.17 with both line-drawings and photographs. Additionally, 11 included studies used clinician directed interventions, 7 used hybrid interventions and no studies used client-centered interventions. The hybrid interventions were all receptive, while the clinician directed interventions included both expressive and receptive labeling. Clinician directed interventions that targeted receptive labeling and used line-drawings or photographs lead to the highest average rate of learning across the studies (line-drawings M = 2.24; photographs M = 2.2). Receptive interventions had a higher rate of learning overall as compared to expressive interventions. Clinician directed interventions that targeted expressive labeling and used photographs lead to the highest rates of learning across the expressive studies (M = 1.56). Finally, interventions were sorted by technology vs. no-technology. Consistent with previous findings, interventions that included technology showed higher rates of learning than those that did not use technology. However, for those that did include technology, line-drawings (Clinician directed M = 4.17; Hybrid M = 0.81) were associated with higher rates of learning than the other stimulus types, followed by photographs and then objects. Conclusions: The results of this review demonstrate the significance of visual stimuli selection during word learning interventions, especially for children with ASD and varying degrees of language impairment. Results suggest that the visual stimulus that promotes the highest rate of learning changes as language abilities increase. Specifically, children who are nonverbal may perform best with objects, children who have minimal verbal skills may perform best with photographs and children with emerging verbal skills may perform best with line-drawings. Additionally, other interventions factors, such as use of technology and target of intervention (expressive or receptive labeling) may impact stimuli selection and rate of learning, such that interventions that include technology lead to higher rates of learning as do interventions that target receptive labeling. Other significant take-aways from this review include identification of the need for standardized reporting of visual stimuli types, child characteristics (i.e. language, ASD severity) and intervention outcomes to better allow for replication and comparison between studies. Future studies should continue investigating visual stimuli as an active ingredient during word learning interventions with children with ASD.
    URI
    https://hdl.handle.net/1808/34767
    Collections
    • Dissertations [4626]

    Items in KU ScholarWorks are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.


    We want to hear from you! Please share your stories about how Open Access to this item benefits YOU.


    Contact KU ScholarWorks
    785-864-8983
    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    785-864-8983

    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    Image Credits
     

     

    Browse

    All of KU ScholarWorksCommunities & CollectionsThis Collection

    My Account

    Login

    Statistics

    View Usage Statistics

    Contact KU ScholarWorks
    785-864-8983
    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    785-864-8983

    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    Image Credits
     

     

    The University of Kansas
      Contact KU ScholarWorks
    Lawrence, KS | Maps
     
    • Academics
    • Admission
    • Alumni
    • Athletics
    • Campuses
    • Giving
    • Jobs

    The University of Kansas prohibits discrimination on the basis of race, color, ethnicity, religion, sex, national origin, age, ancestry, disability, status as a veteran, sexual orientation, marital status, parental status, gender identity, gender expression and genetic information in the University’s programs and activities. The following person has been designated to handle inquiries regarding the non-discrimination policies: Director of the Office of Institutional Opportunity and Access, IOA@ku.edu, 1246 W. Campus Road, Room 153A, Lawrence, KS, 66045, (785)864-6414, 711 TTY.

     Contact KU
    Lawrence, KS | Maps