KUKU

KU ScholarWorks

  • myKU
  • Email
  • Enroll & Pay
  • KU Directory
    • Login
    View Item 
    •   KU ScholarWorks
    • Dissertations and Theses
    • Dissertations
    • View Item
    •   KU ScholarWorks
    • Dissertations and Theses
    • Dissertations
    • View Item
    JavaScript is disabled for your browser. Some features of this site may not work without it.

    The online use of markedness information in L1 and L2 Spanish gender agreement

    Thumbnail
    View/Open
    LopezPrego_ku_0099D_14313_DATA_1.pdf (1.636Mb)
    Issue Date
    2015-12-31
    Author
    Lopez Prego, Beatriz
    Publisher
    University of Kansas
    Format
    131 pages
    Type
    Dissertation
    Degree Level
    Ph.D.
    Discipline
    Linguistics
    Rights
    Copyright held by the author.
    Metadata
    Show full item record
    Abstract
    ABSTRACT The present study focuses on the acquisition and processing of gender agreement by second language (L2) learners of Spanish, whose first language (L1; English) lacks gender. Some L2 theories argue that these learners will not be able to acquire gender, and will have to resort to different strategies to process it in their second language (Hawkins, 2009), particularly in long-distance agreement dependencies (Clahsen & Felser, 2006; Clahsen, Felser, Neubauer, & Silva, 2010). Other theories argue that it is possible for those learners to acquire gender, but they may experience difficulty accessing target gendered forms, due to the computational burden of using a second language (Haznedar & Schwartz, 1997; Prévost & White, 2000). The current study addresses these theories by investigating how native speakers and advanced L2 learners use the gender markedness information (masculine vs. feminine) conveyed by the first element in a long-distance agreement dependency in particular, to process the second agreeing element in the dependency. In addition, it is investigated whether native speakers performing a task under processing burden show similar patterns to L2 learners in their processing of gender agreement (Hopp, 2010; McDonald, 2006; López Prego & Gabriele, 2014). This latter approach attempts to test whether specific error patterns in L2 learners emerge due to processing difficulty, or to a flawed representation of the gender feature. Thus, the study contributes unique data to answer the following questions: whether advanced L2 learners can establish long-distance agreement dependencies; whether they can develop a native-like representation of the gender feature in their L2, when they lack gender in their L1; and whether they can use gender information in a native-like manner in their online processing of agreement. These questions were tested in a self-paced reading task in which a grammaticality judgment was provided after each sentence. The group of native speakers performing under processing burden was additionally asked to decide whether a string of numbers presented before each sentence was the same or different from a string presented after the grammaticality judgment was supplied. The main results of the study showed that the advanced L2 learners tested, like the native speaker control group, were sensitive to gender agreement violations in long-distance agreement dependencies. In addition, both groups revealed a significant facilitation effect from the marked (feminine) feature in their processing of long-distance agreement dependencies, crucially, in grammatical sentences. Finally, the native speakers performing under processing burden showed some weak patterns that nevertheless resembled those in the L2 learner group. Thus, the findings from the present study support theories that posit computational difficulty as the source of agreement variability in L2 learners, and run counter to theories proposing a grammatical deficit in the L2 grammar as the cause of agreement errors in learners.
    URI
    http://hdl.handle.net/1808/21693
    Collections
    • Dissertations [4474]
    • Linguistics Dissertations and Theses [95]

    Items in KU ScholarWorks are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.


    We want to hear from you! Please share your stories about how Open Access to this item benefits YOU.


    Contact KU ScholarWorks
    785-864-8983
    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    785-864-8983

    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    Image Credits
     

     

    Browse

    All of KU ScholarWorksCommunities & CollectionsThis Collection

    My Account

    LoginRegister

    Statistics

    View Usage Statistics

    Contact KU ScholarWorks
    785-864-8983
    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    785-864-8983

    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    Image Credits
     

     

    The University of Kansas
      Contact KU ScholarWorks
    Lawrence, KS | Maps
     
    • Academics
    • Admission
    • Alumni
    • Athletics
    • Campuses
    • Giving
    • Jobs

    The University of Kansas prohibits discrimination on the basis of race, color, ethnicity, religion, sex, national origin, age, ancestry, disability, status as a veteran, sexual orientation, marital status, parental status, gender identity, gender expression and genetic information in the University’s programs and activities. The following person has been designated to handle inquiries regarding the non-discrimination policies: Director of the Office of Institutional Opportunity and Access, IOA@ku.edu, 1246 W. Campus Road, Room 153A, Lawrence, KS, 66045, (785)864-6414, 711 TTY.

     Contact KU
    Lawrence, KS | Maps