KUKU

KU ScholarWorks

  • myKU
  • Email
  • Enroll & Pay
  • KU Directory
    • Login
    View Item 
    •   KU ScholarWorks
    • Dissertations and Theses
    • Dissertations
    • View Item
    •   KU ScholarWorks
    • Dissertations and Theses
    • Dissertations
    • View Item
    JavaScript is disabled for your browser. Some features of this site may not work without it.

    Effects of Neighborhood Density and Noise on Children's Word Learning

    Thumbnail
    View/Open
    Han_ku_0099D_13797_DATA_1.pdf (1.865Mb)
    Issue Date
    2014-12-31
    Author
    Han, Min Kyung
    Publisher
    University of Kansas
    Format
    72 pages
    Type
    Dissertation
    Degree Level
    Ph.D.
    Discipline
    Hearing and Speech
    Rights
    Copyright held by the author.
    Metadata
    Show full item record
    Abstract
    Studies show that words are organized with similarity neighborhoods based on similar sound structure. Some words have many similar sounding words, while others have few. The number of neighbors a word has is called neighborhood density, which is known to influence word learning. Specifically, words with few neighbors are learned more accurately early in training perhaps because these words play a role in triggering the learning of a novel word. In contrast, words with many neighbors are learned more accurately later in training and post training perhaps because these words play a role in the construction of a new lexical representation in long-term memory and in the connection of the newly constructed lexical representation with existing representations (Storkel, Armbrüster, & Hogan, 2006; Storkel, Bontempor, Aschenbrenner, Maekawa, & Lee, 2013; Storkel & Lee, 2011). However, these findings were obtained in a quiet listening condition, providing little information about the effect of the environment where word learning typically takes place. The goal of this study was to examine whether noise alters the effect of neighborhood density on word learning. Seventy-seven typically developing 4- and 5-year-old preschool children were randomly assigned to one of three listening conditions: 0dB, +6dB, and +15dB signal-to-noise ratio (SNR). Sixteen consonant-vowel-consonant nonword-novel object referent pairs were embedded in two stories for training; neighborhood density for the nonwords varied from low to high. Nonword stimuli and audio narrative scripts for stories were digitally mixed with broadband white noise at 0dB, +6dB, and +15dB SNR. Learning was measured using a picture naming task and a referent identification task. Six cycles of story training-measures of learning were completed with two no training points each after the third and sixth measures of learning. Logistic multi-level modeling (MLM) revealed different patterns of word learning depending on the tasks. Only in the naming task, a significant effect of noise and an interaction between noise and neighborhood density were found at +6dB SNR compared to 0dB SNR. Specifically, results showed that (1) word learning was better at 6dB SNR than 0dB SNR; (2) no significant effect of density was found and this non-significance persisted over time. However, the high density advantage started to emerge at +6dB SNR and +15dB SNR; and (3) the difference between +6dB SNR and 0dB SNR was greater as density increased. In addition, in both naming and referent identification tasks, word learning increased over time with significant forgetting of words in the naming task and a trend of memory consolidation in the referent identification when no training was occurred. These results provide the evidence that word learning declines as listening environment worsen. The results indicate that noise hinders children's ability to use lexical representations, which adversely influences the whole process of word learning (i.e., triggering, configuration, and engagement). The results also imply that high density words are more sensitive to listening condition than low density words. In addition, the naming task that requires more detailed lexical representation is more sensitive to noise than the referent identification task.
    URI
    http://hdl.handle.net/1808/18398
    Collections
    • Dissertations [4475]
    • Speech-Language-Hearing Dissertations and Theses [75]

    Items in KU ScholarWorks are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.


    We want to hear from you! Please share your stories about how Open Access to this item benefits YOU.


    Contact KU ScholarWorks
    785-864-8983
    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    785-864-8983

    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    Image Credits
     

     

    Browse

    All of KU ScholarWorksCommunities & CollectionsThis Collection

    My Account

    LoginRegister

    Statistics

    View Usage Statistics

    Contact KU ScholarWorks
    785-864-8983
    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    785-864-8983

    KU Libraries
    1425 Jayhawk Blvd
    Lawrence, KS 66045
    Image Credits
     

     

    The University of Kansas
      Contact KU ScholarWorks
    Lawrence, KS | Maps
     
    • Academics
    • Admission
    • Alumni
    • Athletics
    • Campuses
    • Giving
    • Jobs

    The University of Kansas prohibits discrimination on the basis of race, color, ethnicity, religion, sex, national origin, age, ancestry, disability, status as a veteran, sexual orientation, marital status, parental status, gender identity, gender expression and genetic information in the University’s programs and activities. The following person has been designated to handle inquiries regarding the non-discrimination policies: Director of the Office of Institutional Opportunity and Access, IOA@ku.edu, 1246 W. Campus Road, Room 153A, Lawrence, KS, 66045, (785)864-6414, 711 TTY.

     Contact KU
    Lawrence, KS | Maps