An Investigation into Predictors of Competitive Success  
in a Large-Scale Group Foraging Task 
By 
Copyright 2016 
Bryan Tomes Yanagita 
Submitted to the graduate degree program in the Applied Behavioral Science Department 
and the Graduate Faculty of the University of Kansas in partial fulfillment of the  
requirements for the degree of Master of Arts.  
 
__________________________ 
Chairperson Derek D. Reed 
 
__________________________ 
David P. Jarmolowicz 
 
__________________________ 
Pamela L. Neidert 
Date Defended:  
Wednesday, June 15th, 2016 
 
 
ii 
 
The Thesis Committee for Bryan Tomes Yanagita 
certifies that this is the approved version of the following thesis: 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
An Investigation into Predictors of Competitive Success  
in a Large-Scale Group Foraging Task 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
__________________________ 
Chairperson Derek D. Reed 
 
Date approved: Wednesday, June 15th, 2016 
 
 
 
 
 
iii 
 
Abstract 
Ideal Free Distribution Theory (IFD) (Fretwell & Lucas, 1970) suggests that the allocation of 
organisms to two or more resource sites is a function of the available resources at each site. 
Experimental arrangements involving human participants consist of a series of trials in which 
participants choose between two resource sites with differing resource values. Past research has 
investigated the relationship between delay discounting and performance in an IFD task, noting 
correlations between higher discounting and poorer individual performance. The current study 
sought to investigate the predictive capabilities of various psychological assessments, including 
an individual and group-context delay discounting task. Results demonstrate that the allocation 
of participants conformed to the IFD theory. Additionally, results suggest that, while controlling 
for all other assessments, the individual delay discounting assessment, competitive index score, 
and proportion of trials switched significantly predicted performance. 
Keywords: delay discounting, group context delay discounting, ideal free distribution 
theory, competitive success 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
iv 
 
Acknowledgements 
I would like to thank Drs. Derek Reed, Dave Jarmolowicz, and Pam Neidert for 
dedicating their time, expertise, and support while serving on my Master’s committee.  
I cannot express how thankful I am to earn my Master’s degree from KU, and I owe this 
opportunity to Derek for seeing my potential and being my mentor throughout the process. I will 
always look fondly on my adventure at KU, and am happy that I was able to work with you. You 
have provided me with a life and career path, and I will confidently leave KU knowing that I was 
able to learn from you.  
I have appreciated Pam’s generosity, mentorship, and supervision, as she provided me 
with the opportunity to gain applied experience. Although I am not your graduate student, you 
made time for me and provided guidance during my time at Sunnyside. My experience in the 
CDC has been both challenging and exciting, and I am thankful for what you have taught me. 
I would like to thank Dave for his support and mentorship in classes, and for providing 
feedback on projects along the way. I have appreciated your insight, approachability, and 
willingness to assist me with any and all questions.  
I would like to thank Amel, Brent, Gideon, Alec, Kelsey, and Andi for helping me 
transition to graduate school, supporting my research and GTA positions, and most importantly, 
your friendship. You have all been a large part of my graduate experience, and I will always 
appreciate what you have done for me.  
 Lastly, I am forever grateful for the love and support of my family. Thank you for 
everything. 
 
 
 
 
 
 
v 
 
Table of Contents 
Introduction  1  
Methods  13 
Results  19  
Discussion  22 
References  28 
Tables  33 
Figures  37 
Appendices  44 
  
 
  
 
 
vi 
 
List of Illustrative Materials 
Table Captions 
 
  
Table 1. Breakdown of extra credit earned for the in-class group foraging 
task.  
 
33 
Table 2. 
 
Table 3.  
 
 
Table 4.  
 
Point availability for each resource site across all six conditions. 
 
Correlation matrix for the online survey assessments and 
performance measures from the in-class group foraging task. 
 
Summary of multiple regression analysis for predicting 
individual competitive success (i.e., total points earned during 
the group foraging task).  
 
34 
 
35 
 
 
36 
Figure Captions 
 
  
Figure 1. Natural log k-values across small, medium, and large magnitude 
hypothetical monetary rewards for both the group context and 
individual delay discounting assessments.  
 
37 
Figure 2. Distribution of k-values for both group context and individual 
delay discounting assessments.  
 
38 
Figure 3. The number of people selecting resource site “A” across all 15 
trials for all six conditions. The dashed line on each graph 
indicates the predicted number of individuals selecting resource 
site “A” based on the IFD equation 4.  
 
39 
Figure 4. The proportion of individuals as a function of the proportion of 
available points. The left panel shows the average of the first 
five trials for each condition, the middle panel shows the 
average of the middle five trials for each condition, and the right 
panel shows the average of the last five trials for each condition.  
 
40 
Figure 5. The observed number of individuals that selected resource site 
“A” plotted as a function of the predicted number of individuals 
that selected resource site “A” based on the IFD equation 4. 
Data points show the average of the last five trials for each 
condition.  
 
41 
Figure 6. The proportion of trials switched as a function of the log ratio of 
points earned on the previous trial.  
 
42 
 
 
vii 
 
Figure 7. Data collection method comparison between iClicker total 
switches plotted as a function of paper-pencil switches for each 
participant.  
 
43 
 
 
   
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
viii 
 
Appendix Index 
A) Informed consent form  44 
B) Online survey flow  45 
C) Example individual delay discounting assessment 46 
D) Example group context delay discounting assessment 47 
E) Social Value Orientation assessment  48 
F) Competitiveness index  49 
G) iClicker 2  50 
H) Participant data collection sheet for the in-class group foraging task 51 
I) Group foraging task example trial  52 
J) Condition change slide  55 
K) Example of live feedback during group foraging task trial 56 
 
1 
 
An Investigation into Predictors of Competitive Success 
in a Large-Scale Group Foraging Task 
As a quantitative model of choice (Herrnstein, 1961), the matching law states that the 
relative proportion of responses towards an alternative is equal to (i.e., matches) the relative 
proportion of reinforcement available for that alternative. Typically, matching is assessed by 
presenting two concurrently available response alternatives, each corresponding to a different 
variable interval schedule of reinforcement. All behaving is choice, in that at any instance an 
organism may continue to engage in a response or begin engaging in a number of different 
responses (Herrnstein, 1970). Put simply, the paradigm is representative of the choices that 
organisms continuously encounter. The matching law predicts that the relative rate of 
reinforcement for a given choice alternative dictates the response allocation of an organism, 
where  
     
𝑅1
𝑅2
=  
𝐵1
𝐵2
          Equation 1 
R1 and R2 refer to the relative rate of reinforcement, and B1 and B2 refer to the relative response 
rates. 
Baum (1974) provided a generalized equation of the matching law that accounts for the 
sensitivity to reinforcement of the organism and whether there are biases in the organism’s 
responding. Quantitatively, the generalized matching equation is expressed as  
    log(
𝐵1
𝐵2
) = 𝑠 log(
𝑅1
𝑅2
) +  log(𝑏)            Equation 2  
where B refers to the response alternatives and R refers to the available reinforcement for each 
alternative. Sensitivity, or s, refers to the degree to which an organism allocates its behavior to a 
reinforcing choice alternative that is accounted for by reinforcement. Perfect matching, as 
denoted by s=1.00, states that an organism’s allocation of behavior perfectly matches the 
 
 
2 
 
available reinforcement for each reinforcing alternative. For instance, in a concurrent choice 
arrangement, perfect matching would suggest that twice the behavior would be allocated to the 
choice alternative that has twice the available reinforcement. Undermatching, as denoted by 
s<1.00, suggests that less behavior would be allocated to the response alternative than is 
predicted by the matching law. Conversely, overmatching, as denoted by s>1.00, suggests that 
more behavior would be allocated to the response alternative than predicted by the matching law. 
Lastly, bias (b) refers to any preferences for a response alternative that is unaccounted for by 
reinforcement. A positive bias refers to a preference towards the response alternative associated 
with the numerator of the response portion of equation 2 that is unaccounted by reinforcement, 
while a negative bias indicates a preference towards the denominator that is unaccounted by 
reinforcement.  
 The matching law has been applied to understand the behavioral allocation of a diverse 
range of behaviors including sports (e.g., Alferink, Critchfield, Hitt, & Higgins, 2009; Reed, 
Critchfield, & Martens, 2006), academics (e.g., Reed & Martens, 2008), and social dynamics 
(e.g., Borrero, Crisolo, Tu, Rieland, Ross, Francisco, & Yamamoto, 2007). Although the 
matching law has been helpful in understanding the distribution of an individual’s behavior in a 
variety of contexts, recently, ecological biology has been applied to understand the distribution 
of organisms operating within a group, known as the Ideal Free Distribution (IFD) theory.  
The IFD theory (Fretwell & Lucas, 1970) is an extension of the matching law that 
accounts for the behavior of organisms operating in a group setting, and suggests that the 
allocation of organisms to two or more resource sites is a function of the available resources at 
those sites. There are distinct similarities between both the qualitative and quantitative aspects of 
the matching law and the IFD theory. First, the dependent variable in a matching law paradigm is 
 
 
3 
 
an organism’s behavior, while the dependent variable in an IFD paradigm is the number of 
organisms. Instead of manipulating the relative rate of reinforcement (i.e., matching law) for the 
two different response alternatives, the typical IFD paradigm involves the manipulation of the 
relative amount of resources available at the two different resource sites. Lastly, the quantitative 
models are represented by the same equation; instead of the proportion of behavior as a function 
of reinforcement, it is the proportion of organisms as a function of the available resources.   
Quantitatively, its simplest form states that the proportion of individuals at a resource site 
is equal to the proportion of available resources (Baum & Kraft, 1998):    
                          
𝑁1
𝑁2
=  
𝐴1
𝐴2
                                            Equation 3 
where N refers to the number of individuals at a resource site, and A refers to the available 
resources. Therefore, it is predicted that if there are twice as many resources available at site one 
than site two, there will be twice as many organisms at site one than site two. This result of 
perfectly equal proportion of individuals to resources is referred to as habitat matching (Pulliam 
& Caraco, 1984). As with the matching law, however, there are often deviations from habitat 
matching.  That is, the perfect distribution of organisms to available resources rarely occurs, 
resulting in a need for a generalized version of the equation to account for the deviations from 
the IFD theory. In 1987, Fagan modified the generalized matching equation to account for 
groups of organisms (cf., Baum, 1974; equation 2): 
                                                              log(
𝑁1
𝑁2
) = 𝑠 log(
𝐴1
𝐴2
) + log(𝑏)                            Equation 4 
In the generalized ideal free distribution model, N refers to the number of individuals at a 
resource site. A refers to the amount of resources available at each site. The value, s, refers to the 
sensitivity to differences in the amount of resources available in each resource site. Lastly, b 
 
 
4 
 
refers to the group bias that influences the relative allocation of organisms to each resource site. 
A potential bias, for instance, could be the presence of a predator at a resource site. 
Perfect matching, or habitat matching, is represented by s=1. Undermatching (i.e., s < 
1.00) refers to a situation in which the number of individuals at a resource site is less extreme 
than what is predicted based on the amount of available resources. .  That is, there are less 
individuals at the resource site than predicted by equation one. Conversely, when there are more 
organisms at a resource site than predicted by equation one, the pattern of responding is referred 
to as overmatching (i.e., s > 1.00). Undermatching is the most common finding in the literature, 
with an average s value of 0.70 across a wide range of species (Kennedy & Gray, 1993). These 
data suggest organisms are typically less sensitive to resource allocations than is predicted by the 
IFD theory.  
Initial investigations of the IFD theory originated in ecological and biological research 
aimed at assessing the relative distribution of non-human animals towards two or more resource 
sites in contrived and natural environments (e.g., Milinksi, 1984; Harper, 1982; Abrahams, 
1989), concluding that IFD theory predicted the distribution of organisms. As one example, 
Harper (1982) manipulated the availability of resources (i.e., pieces of white bread) for a group 
of mallard ducks in their natural habitat (i.e., a pond). Both the rate at which resources were 
presented and the amount of resources presented (i.e., weight of bread) were manipulated, 
finding that the distribution of ducks closely approximated the predictions based on the IFD 
theory.  
Although IFD has been experimentally studied with non-humans, there have been 
relatively few investigations into the IFD of humans (e.g., Kraft & Baum, 2001; Kraft, Baum, & 
Burge, 2002; Sokolowski, Tonneau, & Baque, 1999; Madden et al., 2002; Critchfield & 
 
 
5 
 
Atteberry, 2003). Despite the lack of research, the initial data provide support of the IFD theory 
for humans in both experimental and natural settings.  
In a typical human operant IFD arrangement, participants are instructed to make choices 
between two resource sites differing in the amount or rate of rewards provided. There are 
numerous experimental manipulations which result in different levels of conformity to the 
predicted distribution of individuals, including the ability to switch resource sites after seeing 
how many individuals are in each site, the ability to communicate (i.e, verbal behavior), and 
whether it is a discrete trial or free operant arrangement. Participants are told that they are to 
compete for limited resources (i.e., hypothetical points that are typically exchangeable for 
monetary rewards or extra credit) by making a series of choices between one of two resource 
sites, in which they evenly split the resources with however many individuals are at each 
resource site. Each choice is a trial, and there are typically 15-20 trials per condition. Each 
condition is associated with a constant rate or amount of rewards for each site, with most 
experiments employing four to six conditions (including a direct replication condition).  
In 1999, Sokolowski et al. conducted the first experimental investigation of the 
distribution of human participants between two resource sites. Each resource site had a specific 
number of token rewards that were available, and these rewards were evenly split between the 
number of individuals that were at the resource site. The participant that had the most tokens was 
awarded a cash prize for each condition. The ratio of available tokens for each resource site was 
manipulated between each condition. The results of the experiment demonstrated conformity to 
the ideal free distribution theory, with undermatching patterns of distribution being observed 
(s=0.67).  
 
 
6 
 
Madden et al. (2002) conducted a series of three experiments investigating various 
procedures used to assess the ideal free distribution theory. In all three experiments, 12 
undergraduate students served as participants. Participants were instructed to make a series of 
choices between two resource sites of varying undisclosed point values that would be evenly 
split between each other individual in that resource site, and were told that they were to try and 
earn as many points as they could. In the first experiment, a systematic replication of Kraft and 
Baum (2001), participants were instructed to make a choice between red or blue (i.e., each 
resource site), and could then freely switch between resource sites. The second experiment 
followed the same procedure as experiment one, but participants were unable to switch after their 
initial choice and they were instructed not to talk. This was done to ensure that participants made 
decisions based on their own judgements, rather than on what others said. In the third 
experiment, rewards were delivered on concurrent variable time schedules. In this free operant 
arrangement, participants were able to freely walk between resource sites. The first two 
experiments consisted of 5 conditions, each consisting of 20 trials. In the third experiment, each 
of the five conditions consisted of at least 20 minutes of responding and when the behavior of the 
group was deemed stable.  
The results from experiment one of Madden et al. (2002) show undermatching for both 
initial and final choices, with the final choice distribution of responses (s=0.92) much closer to 
ideal matching than initial selections (s=0.40), and virtually no bias for either the initial or final 
selection. Unsurprisingly, these data suggest initial selections were less sensitive to the available 
resources than the final selections, as participants were able to adapt their choices following 
initial selections to adapt to the distribution of individuals at a given resource site.  
 
 
7 
 
The Madden et al. (2002) manuscript noted that participants communicated—thus, they 
may have strategized—in the first experiment. Therefore, experiment two investigated the IFD 
theory without being able to communicate or freely switch between resource sites following their 
initial selections. Results from experiment two demonstrated undermatching (s=0.82) taken from 
the final six trials of each condition (i.e., stability criteria). Interestingly, as a novel analysis, the 
probability of switching as a function of the points earned on a previous trial was plotted for each 
condition. Analysis indicated participants were a) more likely to switch after low point earnings 
(i.e., 6-8 points), b) would likely stay at the resource site for medium point earnings (i.e., 10-18 
points), and c) would be more likely to switch after big-win point earnings (i.e., 20+ points).  
Madden et al. (2002) data suggest the orderly relation between group allocation and 
available resources in a discrete trial format. However, the authors noted that a free operant 
format, in which organisms are able to freely travel between each resource site, is more 
analogous to the natural environment. Therefore, experiment three employed a free operant 
paradigm in which two response alternatives (i.e., two sides of a room) were programmed to 
deliver rewards on concurrently available variable time schedules of reinforcement. Results from 
experiment three demonstrated a correspondence between the predicted allocation of individuals 
(i.e., predicted by the IFD theory) and the obtained number of participants at each resource site. 
These data lend support to the predictive utility of the IFD theory. Additionally, results of the 
final 5-minutes of each condition (i.e., stability criteria) demonstrated undermatching (s=0.71).  
During group foraging tasks, there is variability between participants in regards to 
competitive success. Critchfield & Atteberry (2003) suggested that an individual’s sensitivity to 
delays may account for the variability in these tasks. Conceptually, individuals that are relatively 
sensitive to delays (i.e., self-controlled) may be more likely to sample the rich resource site, 
 
 
8 
 
switch less often, and earn more overall points. Put simply, individuals that respond in more self-
controlled patterns might be expected to demonstrate more competitive success than individuals 
that are relatively insensitive to delays (i.e., impulsive). To assess sensitivity to delays, 
Critchfield & Atteberry (2003) employed a delay discounting assessment in conjunction with a 
group foraging task. 
Delay discounting refers to the decline in value of a reward based on the delay to which 
that reward is received (Odum, 2011). In a typical human operant delay discounting procedure, 
the participant is provided with a series of dichotomous choices between a smaller hypothetical 
rewards delivered immediately versus larger hypothetical rewards delivered after some delay. 
Preference for the smaller reward across trials is representative of impulsive choice, while 
preference towards the larger yet delayed reward across trials is representative of self-controlled 
choice. Steep rates of discounting (i.e., a pattern of responding towards smaller-immediately 
available rewards) has been associated with maladaptive behaviors including obesity, gambling, 
and substance abuse (Epstein, Salvy, Carr, Dearing, & Bickel, 2010; Reynolds, 2006).  
Critchfield and Atteberry (2003) investigated the relation between delay discounting and 
performance in an IFD activity with college undergraduates in a human operant arrangement. 
The rationale for their study was based upon past literature that noted the relationship between 
discounting and sensitivity to reinforcement (Critchfield & Atteberry, 2003), as well as 
differences in the rates of discounting that were correlated with prevalence or intensity of 
impulsive choices (Critchfield & Kollins, 2001). Participants completed a six-page paper-pencil 
delay discounting assessment, in which participants made a series of dichotomous choices 
between $1000 received after a delay, or a smaller amount available now. The initial choice was 
between $1000 delivered after a delay and $500 delivered immediately. If the participant 
 
 
9 
 
selected the larger-delayed reward, the smaller amount decreased on the subsequent trial. 
Conversely, if the participant selected the smaller amount, the smaller amount increased on the 
subsequent trail. The delay was manipulated across each page (i.e., condition), and consisted of a 
1 month, 6 month, 1 year, 3 years, 5 years, and 10 year delays. All rewards and delays were 
hypothetical.  
Participants in the Critchfield and Atteberry (2003) study were unsystematically assigned 
to one of 17 groups, each consisting of 9-12 individuals, to participate in the group foraging task 
(i.e,. IFD paradigm). Each participant was provided with both a red and a blue card used for 
responding during the task, each colored card corresponding with a resource site. Participants 
were instructed to raise either the blue or red card to make a choice. Participants were then told 
that they would be responding to earn hypothetical points exchangeable for extra credit, with the 
points being split evenly between the other participants that were at the same resource site. For 
instance, if there were 100 points available for selecting the red resource site, and if 10 
individuals selected red, they would each split the 100 points for a total of 10 points each. The 
participants were not told how many points each resource site was worth. Each group completed 
4 conditions, with each condition containing 16 trials.  
Critchfield and Atteberry (2003) hypothesized that participants demonstrating steeper 
rates of discounting (i.e., a relative preference towards smaller-immediately available rewards) 
would be (a) more likely to sample the lean resource site, (b) less likely to switch resource sites, 
and (c) earn less overall points (i.e., perform worse in the group foraging task).  
Critchfield and Atteberry’s (2003) results demonstrated undermatching across the 
majority of groups, with group slopes ranging from s=0.36-1.30 (median s=0.72). Switch point 
probability as a function of relative point gain was consistent with the results of Madden et al. 
 
 
10 
 
(2002), in that individuals were likely to switch with low relative point earnings, likely to stay 
with medium relative point earnings, and there was a slight increase in switch probability at 
relatively high point earnings. Additionally, results suggest a slight difference between the most 
and least impulsive individuals, as determined by temporal discounting rates, with the most 
impulsive individuals having less pronounced (i.e., less extreme) switch probabilities. Although 
there were substantially different results for individuals in relation to competitive success (i.e., 
rich patch choices, number of switches, frequency of better outcomes, and total point earnings), a 
correlational analysis suggested that temporal discounting accounted for a significant proportion 
of the variance in the data. There were significantly negative correlations between discounting 
rates and log rich/lean choices, log better/worse choices, and total points earned, while there 
were significant correlations between the number of switches and discounting rates in both 
studies. Simply put, steeper discounters tended to choose the lean resource site more often, select 
the worse option more often, switch more often, and earn fewer overall points. 
The IFD theory provides an experimental and conceptual framework that has potential 
applied relevance. The allocation of individuals as a function of the available resources has been 
observed in naturalistic environments, suggesting that environments may be contrived to 
manipulate the relative distribution of individuals. For instance, in one demonstration of the 
distribution of humans in an undisturbed natural environment, Disma, Sokolowski, and Tonneau 
(2011) observed the distribution of children in Istanbul, Turkey, in relation to the available 
resources (i.e., high traffic areas in which the children could sell water bottles). They found that 
the IFD theory was predictive of the proportion of children towards the available resource sites. 
Put simply, the proportion of children at each intersection matched the proportion of cars at those 
intersections. In this sense, the manipulation of the available resource sites (e.g., location of site, 
 
 
11 
 
rate of available resources, the amount of available resources) may result in changes in the 
proportion of individuals at each resource site. The manipulation of these variables could 
influence issues of societal importance by changing the distribution of individuals to areas that 
are more socially appropriate. For instance, social cohesion (i.e., cooperation between groups of 
people from diverse backgrounds) could be enhanced by increasing the available resources in 
areas that supports these interactions (e.g., community centers, parks).  
Past research has looked at social engagement (Borrero, Crisolo, Tu, Rieland, Ross, 
Francisco, & Yamamoto, 2007; Conger & Killeen, 1974; McDowell & Caron, 2010; McDowell 
& Caron, 2010), but these analyses have only been useful for engagement between two people. 
Despite this, it is important to look at group cohesion. Psychological assessments, including 
social discounting measures (Charlton, Yi, Porter, Carter, Bickel, & Rachlin, 2013) and Social 
Value Orientation (Van Lange, Otten, De Bruin, Joireman, 1997), have been used to investigate 
the relative variables that contribute to group cohesion. These assessments are easy to administer 
in an online survey format, and provide an assessment of an individual’s preference for outcomes 
that are face valid in comparison to a group foraging task (e.g., competition, social dynamics). 
Similar to other behavioral indicators of cooperation (see Hursh and Roma, 2013), IFD can 
provide a quantitative application to group cohesion.  
The purpose of the current study is to systematically extend the previous research by 
Critchfield and Atteberry (2003) by assessing the predictive utility of three different empirically 
validated assessments (i.e., delay discounting, social value orientation, competitiveness index) in 
comparison with performance on a group foraging task. Additionally, in an effort to improve the 
novel discounting assessment in Critchfield and Atteberry (2003), the current study employed 
empirically validated delay discounting assessments. Furthermore, Critchfield and Atteberry 
 
 
12 
 
(2003) only assessed individual discounting, while the current study investigated both individual 
(Kirby, Petry, & Bickel, 1999) and group-context delay discounting (Charlton et al., 2013). 
Lastly, a recent experimental investigation (i.e., Sokolowski, Tonneau, & Cordevant, 2015) 
demonstrated the use of a video game-based data collection tool, finding orderly relations 
between available resources and the distribution of individuals responding. Despite this 
demonstration, the current study provided potential improvements by employing a novel data 
collection tool that allows for automated data collection, live feedback for participants, and can 
support 150+ participants in a single experimental session. 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
13 
 
Methods 
Online Survey 
Participants 
160 undergraduate students (138 women, 20 men, 1 prefer not to disclose, 1 gender non-
conforming) enrolled in an introductory behavioral science course at a large Midwestern 
university served as participants, ranging in age from 19 to 45 years old (M= 21.05, SD= 3.29). 
Participant’s received 0.5% extra credit towards their overall grade in the course for the 
completion of the online survey. The research procedures were approved by the University of 
Kansas’ Human Subjects Committee (HSCL #00002182). 
Materials and Procedure 
Participants were notified via email and an in-class announcement about the availability 
of the online survey. The survey, generated via Qualtrics online survey database, was available 
for 11 days (March 24th to April 3rd, 2015) and required approximately 15 minutes to complete. 
The online survey consisted of demographics, an individual (Kirby et al., 1999) and group 
context delay discounting assessment (Charlton et al., 2013), a Social Value Orientation 
assessment (Van Lange et al., 1997), and the Revised Competitiveness Index (Houston, Harris, 
McIntire, & Francis, 2002). 
The survey began with an informed consent page (see appendix A) and demographic 
questions (e.g., age, gender). Following the completion of the demographic questions, 
participants were randomly and evenly assigned to both the individual or group context delay 
discounting assessment. After completing each of these discounting assessments, participants 
were randomly assigned to the Social Value Orientation and the Competitiveness Index that were 
evenly counterbalanced to mitigate order effects. Lastly, participants were required to type in 
 
 
14 
 
their unique iClicker ID number, allowing the experimenter to match their online survey 
responses with their performance in an in-class group foraging task.  
Multiple-choice Questionnaire: 
The individual delay discounting assessment was adapted from the 27-item Kirby delay 
discounting assessment (Kirby et al., 1999), and consisted of 27 questions regarding decisions 
between smaller-immediate hypothetical monetary amounts and larger hypothetical monetary 
amounts delivered after a specified delay that benefits the responding individual (see appendix 
C). The following assumption was provided prior to the administration of the individual delay 
discounting: 
“For each of the next 27 choices, please mark which hypothetical reward you would 
prefer: the smaller reward today, or the larger reward in the specified number of days. 
While you will not actually receive the rewards, pretend you will actually be receiving 
the amount you indicate and answer honestly.” 
 
The group-context delay discounting assessment (Charlton et al., 2013) followed the same 
format (i.e., same delays and monetary amounts), however, the choices were presented as 
benefiting the responding individual and 9 other students enrolled in the Introduction to Applied 
Behavioral Science course (see appendix D). Participants were provided with the following 
assumption: 
“Imagine you must make decisions that affect you and 9 other classmates in ABSC 100. 
For each of the next 27 choices, please mark which hypothetical reward you would 
prefer: the smaller reward today, or the larger reward in the specified number of days. 
However, the choices that you will make will benefit you and 9 other classmates in 
ABSC 100. While neither you nor each of the classmates will not actually receive the 
rewards, pretend that you all will actually be receiving the amount you indicate and 
answer honestly as if the rewards were real.” 
 
The Social Value Orientation assessment consisted of a 9-item trichotomous choice paradigm 
assessing an individual’s prosocial, individualistic, and competitiveness orientations (Van Lange 
et al., 1997; see appendix E). Participants were presented with three options in which they 
 
 
15 
 
selected hypothetical outcomes for themselves and an unknown stranger. Participants were 
provided with an assumption regarding the task, provided below. 
“In this task we ask you to imagine that you have been randomly paired with another 
person, whom we will refer to simply as the "Other." This other person is someone you 
do not know and that you will not knowingly meet in the future. Both you and the 
"Other" person will be making choices by selecting either the letter A, B, or C. Your own 
choices will produce points for both yourself and the "Other" person. Likewise, the 
other's choice will produce points for him/her and for you. Every point has a value: the 
more points you receive, the better for you, and the more points the "Other" receives, the 
better for him/her. 
      Here's an example of how this task works: 
 
                                    A          B         C 
You get:         480       540     480 
Other gets:     80        280     480 
 
In this example, if you chose A you would receive 480 points and the other would receive 
80 points; if you chose B, you would receive 540 points and the other 280; and if you 
chose C, you would receive 480 points and the other 480. So, you see that your choice 
influences both the number of points you receive and the number of points the other 
receives. Before you begin making choices, please keep in mind that there are no right or 
wrong answers - choose the option that you, for whatever reason, prefer most. Also, 
remember that the points have value: The more of them you accumulate, the better for 
you. Likewise, from the "other's" point of view, the more points s/he accumulates, the 
better for him/her.”  
 
Participants’ pattern of choices was classified as one of three categories. Prosocial involves 
taking a lesser value of points thereby providing the “other” individual with an equal amount of 
points (i.e., choice C in the provided assumptions). Competitive involves taking a lesser amount 
of points thereby providing the “other” individual with a very small amount of points (i.e., choice 
A). Lastly, individualistic involves taking the highest amount of points, thereby providing the 
“other” individual with a medium amount of points (i.e., choice B). If more than four responses 
were for a single pattern of responding (e.g., 5 responses in the prosocial category), the 
individual responded in ways that are designated as social orientation.  
 
 
16 
 
The Revised Competitiveness Index consists of 14-items divided into two subscales; 
enjoyment of competition and contentiousness (Houston et al., 2002; see appendix F), in which 
participants rated themselves on a five-point Likert scale.  
Group foraging task 
Participants  
One week after the online survey was made available, students in the Introduction to 
Applied Behavioral Science course were allowed to participate in an optional in-class extra 
credit opportunity that took place in their normal classroom during a regularly scheduled class 
period. 154 participants completed the in-class portion of the study. Demographic data were not 
taken, although participants provided their unique iClicker IDs following the completion of the 
in-class study, allowing the researcher to compare results obtained from the in-class portion with 
the online survey portion. The amount of extra credit that participants earned was determined by 
their performance in the group foraging task and is shown in Table 1. Performance was based 
upon the amount of accrued hypothetical points during the task.  
Materials and Setting 
The group foraging task took place in a large, 272 seat lecture hall with stadium-style 
seating. Each participant sat facing three large screens that displayed relevant information. 
Participants were instructed to bring their iClicker 2s (https://www.iclicker.com/; see appendix 
G), a remote device that is used for live quiz responding during the regularly scheduled class 
periods. Each iClicker has a power key, arrows for selecting various options, and 5 response keys 
(A-E). For the purpose of the group foraging task, participants were instructed to power on their 
iClickers and only respond using either the “A” or “B” response keys. The iClickers were synced 
with the computer in the classroom, allowing for automated data collection. Each participant was 
 
 
17 
 
provided with the information statement (see Appendix A), instructions, and a data collection 
sheet (see Appendix H) for each condition (six) to record their point earnings for each trial. 
Two undergraduate research assistants provided inter-observer agreement during data 
collection. One research assistant sat next to the researcher and recorded the number of 
individuals that chose either option “A” or “B” during the task on an Excel spreadsheet. The 
Excel spreadsheet was programmed to calculate the point values that the participants earned after 
each trial, by dividing the number of people selecting each option by the pre-determined 
condition values. The condition point values can be found in Table 2. This research assistant 
helped ensure the accuracy of data by checking that the number of responses on the clicker 
software matched what the researcher entered in the feedback spreadsheet. If any errors 
occurred, the research assistant was trained to fix these issues before the feedback was presented 
to the participants. Despite this, there were no reported errors, and the procedures went according 
to plan. The second research assistant helped ensure fidelity by blacking out the trial screens 
while providing feedback (i.e., the number of points and individuals at each resource site). The 
primary investigater recorded any errors in procedural fidelity, however, there were no errors in 
feedback delivery.  
Procedure 
Participants were instructed to read through the information statement prior to the study. 
Following the information statement, the instructions were displayed on the middle screen at the 
front of the lecture hall, and the instructions were read aloud to the participants (see below).  
“We are interested in how people make choices and decisions. Today, you will make a 
series of choices using your iClickers. You will be able to freely choose and change your 
responses within each trial, but when the trial ends, you must record your choice by 
circling either “A” or “B” in the resource selection column on your data sheet. Following 
this, you will record the number of points that you earned on that trial, as well as the 
number of people in each resource site. This information will be provided to you after 
 
 
18 
 
every choice trial. There will be 6 conditions with 15 trials each, totaling 90 trials. Your 
task is to earn as many points as you can, which will then be calculated into extra credit. 
There will be a certain amount of points delivered for each resource site after each trial. 
These points will be divided equally between each person in the zone. For example, if 
there are 5 people in zone “A” and there is 1 point available, then each person in the zone 
will be rewarded (1/5) or .2 points. There will be many choices today, so it is up to you 
how to respond. If you falsify your records to try to earn more points, you will receive a 
score of zero for this task. Yet, if you record your responses accurately, you will receive 
an additional 0.1% extra credit added to your overall final grade. Finally, once the task 
begins, there is no talking allowed. If you are being disruptive during this task, we will 
respectfully ask you to leave the classroom. This is done to ensure that your choice 
responses are based on your own judgments, rather than based on what others say. If you 
have any clarifying questions, now is the time to ask them. We will not answer questions 
or comments during the task.” 
 
After reading the instructional statement, participants were led through two practice trials to help 
participants understand the task. Participants were instructed to make a choice between resource 
sites “A” or “B”, and were told that they had 15 seconds before the trial ended. Immediately after 
this trial, the experimenter provided feedback to the participants via an Excel spreadsheet on the 
screen for the current trial. The information provided consisted of the number of people in each 
resource site (A or B), as well as how many points were earned per person for A and B. 
Participants were then instructed that they had 15 seconds to record this information on their data 
sheets. The purpose of self-recorded data sheets were a) to help promote attending to the task, b) 
to provide an additional layer of feedback, and c) to provide an additional measure of data 
reliability (in addition to the iClicker software and excel spreadsheet).  
As per Madden et al.’s (2002) experiment two, participants were not allowed to talk. Two 
research assistants monitored the class to ensure there were no distractions or interruptions.   
The experiment began following the completion of the practice trials. Participants 
completed 15 trials per condition, with feedback provided after each trial. Additionally, live 
feedback regarding the number of individuals at each resource site was provided via the iClicker 
software during each trial (see appendix K). Participant distribution was presented as a bar graph 
 
 
19 
 
during the live feedback within each trial, with live adjustments automatically made as 
participants selected or switched resource sites. Each trial took roughly 15-seconds to collect 
responses, and roughly 15-seconds to provide feedback. Following the completion of the 
condition (i.e., 15 trials), a condition change slide was presented and read aloud (see appendix J). 
The condition change slide stated that “The number of points allocated to ‘A’ and ‘B’ has now 
changed. It is up to you to determine the best way to earn points”. This process continued until 
all 6 conditions (90 trials) were completed. Following the completion of the 90 trials, participants 
were required to turn in their paper-pencil data sheets to the research assistants and were allowed 
to leave the classroom.  
Results 
Delay Discounting 
Delay discounting results were obtained for 160 participants. Figure 1 shows the natural 
log transformed k-values for small, medium, and large hypothetical monetary magnitudes for 
both individual and group context delay discounting assessments. The individual MCQ delay 
discounting values were natural log transformed and show a mean small k-value of -3.5646 
(SEM=.1200), a mean medium k-value of -4.2374 (SEM=.1290), and a mean large k-value of      
-4.7083 (SEM=.1293). Additionally, for the group-context natural log transformed delay 
discounting values, the average small k-value of -3.7940 (SEM=.1305), the average medium      
k-value of -4.3597 (SEM=.1263), and the mean large k-value of -4.7312 (SEM=.1366). 
Individual MCQ discounting natural log transformed values ranged from -8.74034 to -1.38629, 
with an average k-value of -4.13943 (see figure 2 for the distribution of natural log transformed               
k-values). Group discounting natural log transformed values ranged from -8.74034 to -1.38629, 
with an average k-value of -4.27381. Individual delay discounting and group context delay 
 
 
20 
 
discounting were compared using a 2 (condition) x 3 (magnitude) repeated-measures ANOVA. 
These analyses demonstrated a significant main effect of magnitude (F(2, 318) = 141.3,              
p < .0001), a non-significant main effect of condition (F(1, 159) = 2.584, p = .1099), and a non-
significant interaction (F(2, 318) = 2.335, p = .0985). Overall consistency scores for the 
individual discounting assessment was 96.13%, while the group delay discounting consistency 
scores were 95.81%. 
Competitiveness and Social Values Orientation 
Competitiveness scores were obtained for 159 participants, with an average 
competiveness score of 45.95 (SEM=0.716). The maximum competiveness score of a participant 
was 67, while the minimum score was 15. Social Value Orientation assessments were completed 
by 160 participants. Participants were scored into four different categories based on their pattern 
of responding: Competitive, individualistic, prosocial, or mixed. There were 71 participants that 
were categorized as prosocial, 57 categorized as individualistic, 24 categorized as competitive, 
and 8 categorized as mixed. 
Group Foraging 
Of the 154 participants that completed the in-class group foraging task, data were 
analyzed for 147 participants due to missing data (i.e., no responses). Figure 3 shows group 
foraging data for each condition. The number of people that selected resource site “A” is 
presented across each trial in the group foraging task. As the trials progressed across all 6 
conditions, the number of individuals stabilized near the predicted level of distribution based on 
the IFD theory.  
The relative proportion of individuals choosing resource site “A” vs. resource site “B” as 
a function of the relative proportion of points available at resource site “A” and “B” is 
 
 
21 
 
represented in Figure 4. The left panel indicates the average distribution of individuals across all 
conditions for the first 5 trials (y=.5872x+.0606, r2=.9590), the middle panel represents the 
average distribution of individuals across all conditions for the middle 5 trials (y=.8599x-.0040, 
r2=.9970), and the right panel represents the average distribution of individuals across all 
conditions for the last 5 trials (y=.9626x+.0049, r2=.9993). Each data point is representative of 
one condition, totaling 6 data points per trial block (i.e., five conditions plus one direct 
replication condition). To assess whether there was a difference between the first five, middle 
five, and last five trials in the group matching plot, a comparison was conducted between a free 
y-intercept and shared yet constrained y-intercept. Constraining a shared y-intercept between the 
three regression lines showed significantly different slopes F(2,14)=28.94, p<.0001. 
Additionally, while allowing a non-constrained y-intercept for the three regression lines, the 
slopes were found to be significantly different F(2,12)=25.65, p<.0001. 
Figure 5 shows the average number of individuals observed in resource site “A” across 
the last five trials of each condition were plotted as a function of the predicted number of 
individuals based on the ideal free distribution equation (see equation 3). Linear functions 
accounted for 99.91% of the variance in the data, with a best fit line of y=0.973x + 2.472.  
Figure 6 shows the probability of switching resource sites, defined as the proportion of 
individuals that switched resource sites as a function of the proportion of points earned on the 
previous trial. The x-axis represents the log-transformed proportion of points earned on the 
selected resource site compared to the points earned in the other resource site. The y-axis depicts 
probability of switching. Each data point represents an instance in which a point value was 
provided and at least one person switched.  
 
 
22 
 
Correlations between individual discounting, group discounting, competitiveness 
assessment, Social Value Orientation, and performance on the group foraging task (i.e., total 
points earned, number of switches) are compared in Table 3. There was a statistically significant 
relationship between individual and group discounting (r(159) = 0.710, p < .001), and individual 
discounting and total points earned (r(159) = 0.186, p < .05). There were no other statistically 
significant relationships between these metrics. 
Predictive Utility 
Table 4 shows results from a multiple regression analysis assessing the predictive utility 
of the psychological and behavioral independent variables that were assessed (i.e., individual 
delay discounting, group context delay discounting, competitiveness index, SVO) on the 
dependent variable of total points earned. Since the purpose of the current study was to assess 
competitive success, the dependent variable of total points was selected. Results indicated that, in 
combination, the proportion of trials switched (β = -6.366, t(152) = -3.177), p = .002), individual 
delay discounting (β = -0.450, t(152) = -2.152, p = .034), and the competitiveness index (β = 
.053, t(152) = 2.301, p = .023) were statistically significantly predictive, while controlling for all 
other variables.  
Discussion 
Delay Discounting 
 Overall, there was a magnitude effect for both individual and group-context delay 
discounting that significantly differed between magnitudes (i.e., small, medium, large). There 
were no condition differences observed between the individual and group context delay 
discounting assessments, despite condition differences in past research (e.g., Bickel, 
Jarmolowicz, Mueller, Franck, Carrin, and Gatchalian, 2012). However, there may be limitations 
 
 
23 
 
in the current study that could account for the inconsistences in these findings. One potential 
explanation could be that the social distance of the group in Bickel et al.’s (2012) study was 
greater than in the current study. In this sense, the student participants from the introductory 
course in the current study may have had a closer social proximity than the groups used in Bickel 
et al.’s (2012) study. Additionally, our group context discounting assessment differed from the 
traditional group context (i.e., “me-we”) assessment. Traditionally, “me-we” discounting 
provides the assumption that individuals are responding for hypothetical rewards for themselves 
and a group of anonymous individuals. However, in order to assess the social cohesiveness of 
our group foraging task (i.e., introduction to behavioral science students), the assumptions were 
adapted to include classmates in the group context. These differences in the group context 
assessment may account for the inconsistencies between studies.    
There was a similar range of values for both the individual and group context delay 
discounting assessments which allowed for comparison between the metrics, showing a 
statistically significant correlation between both the individual and group context delay 
discounting assessments.  
 There were a range of values and categories for the competitiveness and social values 
orientation assessments, respectively, allowing for comparisons between these metrics. 
Ideal Free Distribution Theory  
 Group foraging was described by the IFD theory, accounting for up to 99.91% of the 
variance in the data. Undermatching was observed, suggesting that changes in the number of 
individuals at a resource site were less extreme than changes in the available resources at that 
site. For each condition, as trials progressed, the distribution of participants got closer to ideal 
matching. That is, as participants experienced resource allocation in each condition, they 
 
 
24 
 
distributed themselves in a way that closely matched the proportion of available resources. This 
finding is consistent with past research, in that as participants experience the contingencies 
associated with a resource site selection, and are able to freely switch between resource sites 
within a trial, their distribution was better accounted for by the IFD theory (e.g., Madden et al., 
2002). Additionally, as seen in Sokolowski et al. (2015), there was a high degree of 
correspondence between the average observed and predicted number of individuals selecting 
each resource site across the last five trials of each condition (R2 = .9991).  
 The probability of switching resource sites, as defined by the proportion of individuals 
that switched resource sites following trials of varying point earnings, followed a similar pattern 
as found in past research (i.e., Critchfield and Atteberry, 2003; Madden et al., 2002). Participants 
that experienced lower point values relative to other resource sites were more likely to switch 
resource sites, while individuals that experienced medium point values relative to other resource 
sites tended to stay. The “big-win” pattern of responding, wherein individuals that earned 
relatively high point values relative to the other resource site, had an increased likelihood of 
switching sites.  
 Critchfield and Atteberry (2003) investigated the relationship between delay discounting 
and individual success in a group foraging task, hypothesizing that individuals that demonstrate 
steeper discounting would perform worse than individuals that demonstrate shallower 
discounting. Results from their study showed more impulsive participants tended to choose the 
lean patch more often, select the worse of the two resource sites more often, switch more often 
between resource sites, and earn fewer overall points, while compared to the other more self-
controlled participants. Despite the same rationale for studying the relationship between delay 
discounting and the IFD theory, the current results slightly differ. In our study, there was a 
 
 
25 
 
significant positive correlation between individual discounting and total points earned, 
suggesting that the more impulsive individuals (as defined by steeper discounting) earned more 
overall points. Additionally, a multiple regression analysis showed a predictive negative 
correlation between total points earned for individual discounting, and the proportion of trials 
switched, and a predictive positive correlation between total points and scores on the 
competitiveness index, while controlling for all other assessments.  However, despite these 
differences in the obtained results between these studies, it should be noted that there were 
differences in the procedures used. For instance, our study employed two empirically validated 
discounting assessments; a standard 27-item MCQ assessment and an adapted group-context 
assessment (i.e., Kirby et al., 1999; Charlton et al., 2013). Additionally, in Critchfield and 
Atteberry’s (2003) group foraging task, participants were not allowed to switch within trials. 
During the current study, participants were allowed to freely switch between resources sites 
within a 15-second trial. As past research has shown, experimental manipulations to the ability to 
switch within a trial, the amount of feedback provided, and other procedural variations (e.g., the 
ability to talk, free-operant vs. discrete trial arrangement) all result in varying levels of 
conformity to the IFD theory (e.g., Critchfield & Atteberry, 2003; Madden et al., 2002; 
Sokolowski et al., 2015; Kraft & Baum, 2001). Therefore, the difference in experimental 
arrangements could have accounted for the differences in findings between the current study and 
Critchfield & Atteberry (2003).  
 Despite finding a statistically significant positive correlation between standard 
discounting and total points earned, and a multiple-regression analysis indicating a predictive 
correlation between group context discounting and the proportion of switches, there may be other 
assessments or processes that could better account for the IFD performance data. These data may 
 
 
26 
 
support the notion that a single pattern of responding or assessment may not easily explain 
performance in this group foraging task (Critchfield & Atteberry, 2003).  
 These data suggest that there is variation in individual choice patterns while operating in 
a group foraging context. That is, despite individuals operating under the same group foraging 
contingencies, there is variation in their individual performance such that some participants 
maximize their earned resources while others earn relatively few total resources. Understanding 
this variability through psychological and behavioral assessments may help us understand why 
organisms are more or less sensitive to group contingencies.  
 Future research could investigate other behavioral processes, alone or in combination, to 
see whether there are better predictors of performance in the group foraging task. Additionally, 
the effects of group placement are an avenue for future research. That is, to date, group 
placement in IFD tasks have been largely arbitrary. However, it may be that because 
performance in a group foraging task is dependent on both the available resources and the other 
foragers, the arrangements of groups could influence individual performance. For instance, after 
identifying the correlation between standard discounting and total points earned, future research 
could compare performance between highly impulsive groups, more self-controlled groups, or 
groups that are a mix of more and less impulsive individuals. By better understanding how and 
why individuals perform in competitive group contexts, and by understanding the interactions 
when group placement is a variable, we could better inform group placement in societal contexts 
(e.g., military settings, work settings, school settings) and maximize earned resources.  
In the group foraging experimental arrangement, manipulations of resource availability 
resulted in proportional changes in the number of individuals at each resource site. These data 
supported the IFD theory, suggesting that by changing resource availability (e.g., amount, 
 
 
27 
 
quality, frequency of delivery), there would be changes in the allocation of individuals at these 
resource sites. Although the group foraging task represents a simplistic paradigm, if these results 
are conceptually scaled up, it suggests that the theory could be applied to the built environment. 
For instance, in areas that are have fewer resources allocated to positive outlets (e.g., libraries, 
parks, school programs, athletics), and more resources allocated towards negative outlets (e.g., 
gang activity, unhealthy foods, drugs), we will likely see, a higher proportion of individuals 
sampling the negative outlet resource sites. However, as suggested by the IFD theory, by 
manipulating the availability of resources there will be corresponding changes in the distribution 
of individuals, thus influencing the likelihood that these individuals sample negative outlets. 
There are a few potential limitations that should be noted. As described in the Method 
section, data were obtained using both paper-pencil self-recorded methods and the electronic 
automated iClicker software. Following the collection of data, there were unanticipated issues 
associated with each data collection method. Paper-pencil data demonstrated inaccurate records 
of point values and resource site selections, but each trial was filled in for each participant. 
Conversely, the automated iClicker software recorded accurate records of point values and 
resource site selections, but there were missing data for some participants. Presumably, this was 
because either the software did not record a response from a participant, or the participant did not 
make a response (but recorded it on their data sheet). Therefore, the data from both the paper-
pencil and iClicker software were plotted and compared (see figure 3), demonstrating a 
statistically significant correlation between these values (r(145) = 0.751, p<.0001). Additionally, 
a best-fit line was also plotted (y = .913X + 2.686), with an R2 value of .848. Outliers that were 
identified were excluded from the correlational and best-fit line analyses, but are plotted as red 
dots on figure 3. The electronic iClicker data were used throughout all analyses because they 
 
 
28 
 
were automated, eliminated self-report errors, and were used for immediate feedback during the 
group foraging task.  
Another potential limitation is that the number of participants that completed the online 
survey did not match the number of participants that completed the in-class group foraging task. 
Because these tasks were offered as extra credit options, they were not required and thus 
individuals could opt out of one or both of the research activities. However, given the large 
sample sizes of each aspect of the current study, there were enough participants that completed 
both the in-class and online tasks to warrant statistical analyses. Any data that were used to 
compare the online survey results with the group foraging task were only for participants that 
completed both components of the study.  
Overall, the current study sought to investigate whether several commonly used 
behavioral and psychological assessments were predictive of individual performance in a large-
scale group foraging task. Notably, individual competitive success (i.e., total points earned in the 
group foraging task) was statistically significantly predicted by the proportion of trials switched, 
individual delay discounting, and total competitiveness index score, when controlling for all 
other assessments. As a group, the distribution of participants was well described by the IFD 
theory. 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
29 
 
References 
Abrahams, M. V. (1989). Foraging guppies and the ideal free distribution: The influence of  
information on patch choice. Ethology, 82, 116-126. 
Alferink, L. A., Critchfield, T. S., Hitt, J. L., & Higgins, W. J. (2009). Generality of the matching  
law as a descriptor of shot selection in basketball. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,  
42(3), 595-608. 
Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: bias and  
undermatching. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 22(1), 231-242. 
Baum, W. M., & Kraft, J. R. (1998). Group choice: Competition, travel, and the ideal free  
distribution. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69(3), 227-245. 
Bickel, W. K., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., Franck, C. T., Carrin, C., & Gatchalian, K. M. 
 (2012). Altruism in time: Social temporal discounting differentiates smokers from 
 problem drinkers. Psychopharmacology, 224, 109-120.  
Borrero, J. C., Crisolo, S. S., Tu, Q., Rieland, W. A., Ross, N. A., Francisco, M. T., &  
Yamamoto, K. Y. (2007). An application of the matching law to social dynamics. Journal  
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(4), 589-601. 
Charlton, S. R., Yi, R., Porter, C., Carter, A. E., Bickel, W., & Rachlin, H. (2013). Now for me,  
later for us? Effects of group context on temporal discounting. Journal of behavioral  
decision making, 26(2), 118-127. 
Clicker & Audience Response Systems - iClicker. (n.d.). Retrieved May 09, 2016, from  
 
https://www1.iclicker.com/ 
Conger, R., & Killeen, P. (1974). Use of concurrent operants in small group research: A  
demonstration. Pacific Sociological Review, 399-416. 
 
 
30 
 
Critchfield, T. S., & Atteberry, T. (2003). Temporal discounting predicts individual competitive  
success in a human analogue of group foraging. Behavioural Processes, 64(3), 315-331. 
Critchfield, T. S., & Kollins, S. H. (2001). Temporal discounting: Basic research and the analysis  
of socially important behavior. Journal of applied behavior analysis, 34(1), 101-122. 
Disma, G., Sokolowski, M. B., & Tonneau, F. (2011). Children's competition in a natural setting:  
evidence for the ideal free distribution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(6), 373-379. 
Epstein, L. H., Salvy, S. J., Carr, K. A., Dearing, K. K., & Bickel, W. K. (2010). Food 
 
reinforcement, delay discounting and obesity. Physiology & Behavior, 100(5), 438-445. 
Fagan, R. (1987). A generalized habitat matching rule. Evolutionary Ecology, 1, 5–10. 
Fretwell, S. D., & Lucas, H. L., Jr. (1970). On territorial behavior and other factors influencing  
habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheoretica, 19, 16–36.  
Harper, D. G. C. (1982). Competitive foraging in mallards: “Ideal free ducks.” Animal  
Behaviour, 30(2), 575-584. 
Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and Absolute strength of Response as a Function of Frequency  
of Reinforcement, 2. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 4(3), 267-272. 
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the Law of Effect. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior,  
13(2), 243-266. 
Houston, J., Harris, P., McIntire, & Francis, D. (2002). Revising the competitiveness index using  
factor analysis. Psychological Reports, 90(1), 31-34. 
Hursh, S. R., & Roma, P. G. (2013). Behavioral economics and empirical public policy. Journal  
of the experimental analysis of behavior, 99(1), 98-124. 
Kennedy, M., & Gray, R. D. (1993). Can ecological theory predict the distribution of foraging  
animals? A critical analysis of experiments on the ideal free distribution. Oikos, 158-166. 
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for  
 
 
31 
 
delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental psychology:  
General, 128(1), 78. 
Kraft, J. R., & Baum, W. M. (2001). Group choice: The ideal free distribution of human social  
behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76(1), 21-42. 
Kraft, J. R., Baum, W. M., & Burge, M. J. (2002). Group choice and individual choices:  
modeling human social behavior with the Ideal Free Distribution. Behavioural processes,  
57(2), 227-240. 
Madden, G. J., Peden, B. F., & Yamaguchi, T. (2002). Human group choice: Discrete-trial and  
free-operant tests of the Ideal Free Distribution. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of  
Behavior, 78(1), 1-15. 
McDowell, J. J., & Caron, M. L. (2010). Matching in an undisturbed natural human  
environment. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 93(3), 415-433. 
McDowell, J. J., & Caron, M. L. (2010). Bias and undermatching in delinquent boys’ verbal  
behavior as a function of their level of deviance. Journal of the experimental analysis of  
behavior, 93(3), 471-483. 
Milinksi, M. (1984). Competitive resource sharing: an experimental test of a learning rule for  
ESSs. Animal Behavior, 32, 233-242. 
Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay discounting: I'm a k, you're a k. Journal of the experimental analysis  
of behavior, 96(3), 427-439. 
Pulliam, H. R., & Caraco, T. (1984). Living in groups: is there an optimal group size. In: Krebs,  
J. R. and Davies, N. B. (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach, 3, 122- 
147. 
Reed, D. D., Critchfield, T. S., & Martens, B. K. (2006). The generalized matching law in elite  
 
 
32 
 
sport competition: Football play calling as operant choice. Journal of Applied Behavior  
Analysis, 39(3), 281-297. 
Reed, D. D., & Martens, B. K. (2008). Sensitivity and bias under conditions of equal and unequal  
academic task difficulty. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41(1), 39-52. 
Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use  
 
and gambling. Behavioural pharmacology, 17(8), 651-667. 
 
Sokolowski, M. B. C., Tonneau, F., & i Baqué, E. F. (1999). The ideal free distribution in  
humans: An experimental test. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 6(1), 157-161. 
Sokolowski, M. B., Tonneau, F., & Cordevant, M. A. (2015). A portable system for studying  
discrete‐trial group choice. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 103(2), 419- 
426. 
Van Lange, P. A., De Bruin, E., Otten, W., & Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial,  
individualistic, and competitive orientations: theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of  
personality and social psychology, 73(4), 733. 
 
  
 
 
33 
 
Table 1. 
Percentile Rank Extra Credit 
Earned 
100-75 1.50% 
74-50 1.25% 
49-25 1.00% 
24-0 0.75% 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
34 
 
Table 2. 
Condition Points A Points B Ratio 
1 80 40 2:1 
2 20 100 1:5 
3 100 20 5:1 
4 40 80 1:2 
5 60 60 1:1 
6 20 100 1:5 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
35 
 
Table 3. 
 Competitiveness 
Score 
Group 
Discounting 
Individual 
Discounting 
Total 
Points 
Earned 
Proportion 
of Trials 
Switched 
Competitiveness  ---     
Group 
Discounting 
0.836 ---    
Individual 
Discounting 
-0.062 
 
 
0.710*** 
 
---   
Total Points 
Earned 
0.144 
 
0.023 
 
 
0.186* 
 
---  
Proportion of 
Trials Switched 
-0.089 
-0.140 
 
-0.051 
 
-0.121 
 
--- 
*p<.05 
**p<.01 
***p<.001 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
36 
 
Table 4. 
 
DV = Total Points β SE t p 
Intercept  68.422 1.476 46.342 .000 
Prop. Trials Switched -6.336 1.994 -3.177 .002 
Competitiveness .053 .023 2.301 .023 
Individual Discounting -.450 .209 -2.152 .034 
Group Context Discounting .303 .191 1.585 .116 
Prosocial SVO     
Individualistic SVO -.121 .444 -.272 .786 
Mixed SVO .407 .867 .469 .640 
Competitive SVO -.669 .622 -1.077 .284 
R2 = .187 (Adj. R2= .134) 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
37 
 
 
Figure 1.  
 
 
 
38 
 
 
Figure 2.  
 
 
 
 
 
 
39 
 
 
Figure 3. 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
40 
 
 
Figure 4.  
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
41 
 
 
Figure 5. 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
42 
 
 
Figure 6.  
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
43 
 
 
Figure 7. 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
44 
 
Appendix A 
 
 
 
 
45 
 
Appendix B 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
46 
 
Appendix C 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
47 
 
Appendix D 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
48 
 
Appendix E 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
49 
 
Appendix F 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
50 
 
Appendix G 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
51 
 
Appendix H 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Cond. 1 Last 4 digits of KU I.D.: 
Trial # Resource Selection # of Points Earned # of People in "A" # of People in "B"
1 A                B
2 A                B
3 A                B
4 A                B
5 A                B
6 A                B
7 A                B
8 A                B
9 A                B
10 A                B
11 A                B
12 A                B
13 A                B
14 A                B
15 A                B
 
 
52 
 
Appendix I 
 
 
 
 
53 
 
 
 
 
54 
 
 
 
 
 
 
55 
 
Appendix J 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
56 
 
Appendix K